• 検索結果がありません。

An Essay on the Interrelation between Meaning and Grammar in English Teaching in Second-Language Learning

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

シェア "An Essay on the Interrelation between Meaning and Grammar in English Teaching in Second-Language Learning"

Copied!
7
0
0

読み込み中.... (全文を見る)

全文

(1)

An Essay on the Interrelation

between Meaning and Grammar in English Teaching in

Second-Language Learning

Noriyoshi UEDA

Abstract

This paper is not a treatise but an enlightening essay on English teaching in second-language learning. This paper presents some ideas on English teaching in second-language learning and explores theories that may validate them, with the long-term goal of helping to reconstruct English teaching in Japan. First, the importance of the integration of semantic aspects and syntactic ones, as a psychological factor, in English classes in Japan is suggested through outlining the concept of interlanguage. Next, construction grammar, where grammar is taken as an interface between form and

meaning, is featured since it is based on the

transition theory, one of the cognitive linguistic approaches, primarily concerned with modeling the developmental processes of language acquisition. This is unlike the property theory which is primarily concerned with modeling the nature of the language system. Finally, the author reaches a certain theoretical basis in the preliminary findings to an actual verification of some ideas on the integration between meaning and grammar in English teaching in second-language learning from the viewpoint of a cognitive linguistic approach.

1. Introduction

The author attempted to advance a proposal to make English teaching in Japan more effective by giving critical consideration to generative

grammar, which has been the mainstream of linguistics for the last fifty years, in a separate paper (see Ueda, 2008). The following idea is based upon the nature of linguistic competence in the process of language acquisition:

It is with the holding of some images in the brain that the production of language originates, which is an essential factor as the biological requirement for the human species in order to live. They put these images together by using a syntactic

structure, which is a secondary factor of the

social requirement, which is arranged in a particular order or pattern among various cultures. Therefore, if teachers keep in mind such an utterance operation that is common to human beings, but is unfound in any other species, and integrate the former with the latter, language teaching can be more effective while also taking the social environment into account.

The above is, in part, based upon the following teaching attitude from the field of second language acquisition:

Since language exists to embody the images in the brain, these images take precedence over all other things in a second-language classroom. Grammar, or syntactic structure, is merely a means to organize more complex images. It is imperative to integrate both the “meaning” and the “form”. The teacher should therefore pay much attention to such

(2)

19巻

instruction that leads learners to understand the notion appropriate to each developing stage of learning, while explaining how they interrelate.

This paper explores theories that may validate the above ideas discussed in Ueda (2008), with the long-term goal of helping to reconstruct English teaching in Japan.

2. Interlanguage

In this section, the author discusses how to treat some of the strange sentences which second language learners often produce in the learning process.

There are two perspectives regarding second language acquisition: a teaching perspective and a learning one. According to Selinker (1972), the teaching perspective is relevant to “what has to be done by the teacher in order to help the learner achieve learning”. The learning perspective, on the other hand, is relevant to “the process of the attempted learning of a second language, successful or not”. Though it is important for the teacher in a second language classroom to consider both perspectives, many English teachers in Japan do not have an appreciation of the learning perspective. Any incorrect expressions, for example, tend to be treated as mere errors, without consideration of the psychological process affecting the answers. This lack of regard for student effort and of consideration for the learning perspective gradually reduces students’ desire to learn. If wrong answers could be regarded as a part of the learning process, rather than simply errors, Japanese English classes would improve. In order to help teachers appreciate that errors are part of the learning process, they could refer to Selinker’s (1972) concept of “interlanguage”. Interlanguage is the learner’s own language, though with a different linguistic system from target one, which often develops in the learning process. This “interlanguage”

preserves some features of both the native language and the target one (cf. Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia). Adopting this concept, any expressions, including incorrect ones, should be thought of as a vital part of second language acquisition.

The concept of interlanguage, which offers a general account of how second language acquisition takes place, incorporates elements from mentalist theories of linguistics as well as from cognitive psychology (Ellis, 1997). It also provides suitable material for the study here, as can be seen using Selinker’s theories (1972). Whenever the learner attempts to acquire the second language, he or she understands or produces each sentence of the target language “with the aid of theoretical constructs which assume the major features of the psychological structure” (Selinker, 1972: 211) in his or her brain. Such “interlingual identifications” (Weinreich, 1953) that bilinguals make then integrate into the learner’s brain. Assuming “interlingual identifications” exist, this paper can utilize the concept of latent language structure, the closest thing in literature to the concept of latent psychological structure (Selinker, 1972), which is transformed by the infant into the realized structure of a particular grammar in accordance with certain developmental stages (Lenneberg, 1967).

Thus, if an interlanguage, which is an emerging linguistic system that has been developed by a learner of a second language (cf. Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia), can be considered as a latent psychological structure existing in the learner’s brain, the following proposal may be possible:

Each incorrect expression is an example of interlanguage, which comes from a diverse psychological construction between the native language and the target one, which is influenced by various environmental factors. One of the major factors in errors is a psychological reaction. It is therefore necessary to consider the psychological

(3)

causes underlying each incorrect expression.

3. Cognitive Linguistic Approach

The preceding section dealt with the concept of interlanguage. The reason this concept was discussed in depth was to call attention to a kind of insensitive instruction that has often occurred in English classes in Japan. The English teacher in Japan is likely to interpret the cause of error expressions through syntactic aspects rather than semantic ones. It is true that this kind of approach is approved in terms of generative grammar, which focuses the attention on inferring the nature of restricted linguistic knowledge of the syntactic structure. However, this approach limits the language acquisition and linguistic competence in the classroom. We should therefore consider the interrelationship between form and meaning.

Over the last few decades, the main focus of interest and study of particular linguists, called innatists (including many Japanese researchers), has been on language knowledge. Following Chomsky (1965), the centre of language knowledge is grammar, or syntactic structure. It is the business of theoretical linguistics to study and model underlying language knowledge, semantic factors are not regarded as essential, rather than the performance data of actual utterances that people have produced. There is a so-called property theory which is primarily concerned with modeling the nature of the language system that is to be acquired. On the other hand, a transition theory, which is primarily concerned with modeling the developmental processes of language acquisition, has been gradually evolving since the 1980s. Though the interrelationship between form and

meaning is ignored in the first approach, that

interrelationship is fundamental to the second approach, which seeks to understand how the human brain processes and learns new

information (Mitchell and Myles, 2004: 5-12). There are the various theoretical hypotheses in a cognitive linguistic approach. If it is taken into consideration that construction grammar arose as a response to the model of grammatical knowledge proposed by the various versions of generative grammar from the 1960s to at least the 1980s, and while considering the usage-based model which is proposed for language use, language acquisition and language change contrasts with the traditional structuralist and generative models of grammatical representation (Croft and Cruse, 2004:225-290). Both of these theoretical hypotheses, construction grammar and the usage-based model, play an important role in the research on the nature of language knowledge and the acquisition/performance of linguistic competence. In the former, grammar is taken as an interface between form and meaning, where featuring the peripheral factors (prioritized especially among innatists especially) in the particular sentence patterns (constructions) as the integration of both because of much recent concern about semantic and pragmatic aspects (Goldberg, 1995; Ohori, 2002). The latter is the theory of the language acquisition process (i.e. emerging linguistic competence) based on incorporating the prototype after schematizing it using daily language experiences into a categorical hierarchy (Langacker, 2000; Hayase and Horita, 2005), unlike universal grammar theory in which linguistic competence itself is focused on a system with abstract structures. In this section, the author outlines the first theoretical hypothesis: construction grammar. (The other will be dealt with in a separate paper.)

Construction Grammar

According to Croft (2001: 15), “construction grammar arose out of a concern to analyze a problematic phenomenon for the componential model, namely idioms (Fillmore et al. 1988). Idioms are linguistic expressions that are syntactically and/or semantically idiosyncratic

(4)

19巻

in various ways, but are larger than words and hence cannot simply be assigned to the lexicon without some special mechanism”. In generative grammar (Chomsky, 1965: 25), where much attention is paid to syntactic theory and the problem of developing an account of the innate linguistic theory based on the internal organization of the syntactic component (Croft and Cruse, 2004: 229) is set, semantic reference does not appear to affect the manner in which the acquisition of syntax proceeds (Chomsky, 1965: 33). It follows that in generative grammar, the mainstream of linguistics, idiosyncratic expressions, like idioms, have been excluded from the study of language as not a part of the “core” but as a “periphery”. In construction grammar, on the other hand, using “the insight that language is a repertoire of more or less complex patterns -CONSTRUCTIONS- that integrate form and meaning in a conventionalized, and often non-compositional, way” (Fried), language is analyzed as a pairing of syntactic and semantic aspects.

The author’s discussion of construction grammar here focuses on some specific patterns. Goldberg (1995; 1999) proposed the existence of argument structure constructions, which are essential to the production of clause expressions in language. The author here examines four argument structure constructions: transitive motion (hereafter TM), intransitive motion (hereafter IM), caused motion (hereafter CM), and resultative (hereafter R). The conceptual features (usage examples) of these constructions are as follows (Goldberg, 1995):

TM⇒X acts on Y (I play the guitar.) IM⇒ X moves to Y (The fly buzzed into the

room.)

CM⇒ X causes Y to move Z (Pat sneezed the napkin off the table.)

R ⇒ X causes Y to become Z (She kissed him unconscious.)

These constructions possess general purpose

verbs as the typical ones, respectively. These verbs designate meanings that are remarkably similar to the meanings associated with argument structure constructions (ibid: 40). The initial meanings of these verbs are as follows (ibid: 41):

TM⇒do: ‘perform an action’ IM⇒go: ‘move’

CM⇒put: ‘cause to be or go in some place’ R ⇒ make: ‘construct; produce; cause

some state to come into being or be produced’

At the same time, constructions could possibly be defined as ordered triples of form, meaning, and context (ibid: 229). In this case, “grammar” is equivalent to “argument structure construction”. “Meaning” is based on the specific feature above in the syntactic frame as well as derived from the initial meaning, like the above, in the lexical frame. “Context” is, needless to say, related to the social environment. These three factors are indispensable to structuring language. Argument structure constructions are, above all, important and are taken to be the basic units of language (ibid: 4). For example, “Children with Specific Language Impairment” rely heavily on the same set of light verbs, including go, get, do, put, and make, in their production of sentences. Sentences such as I’m

doing two balloons commonly replace the more

appropriate I’m using/playing with/bouncing

two balloons (ibid: 42). In this case, the fact that

one verb is substituted for various verbs in a particular construction means that the syntactic frame affects the semantic aspects. That is, the syntactic frame plays a complementary role facilitating the understanding of meaning in a certain situation. Thus, these three factors complement each other. One can then assume that to embody an image visualized in the brain for the first time, an argument structure which is derived from the semantic frame which is fused with the lexicon is set and a suitable sentence

(5)

for the context is produced.

Let us consider the following examples (Goldberg, 1995, 1999; Ohori, 2002):

Pat sneezed.

Pat sneezed the foam off the cappuccino. Pat sneezed a terrible sneeze.

Pat sneezed her nose red. Pat sneezed her way to the room.

Since the verb, sneeze, is intransitive, the above second, third, and fourth sentences are not grammatically acceptable. They are accepted only when they have such specific words such as “off”, “terrible” or “red”. However, the following sentence is unacceptable:

* Pat winked the foam off the cappuccino.

The reason why it is not accepted is based on the difference of qualities between “sneeze” and “wink” (Ohori, 2002: 136-138). As a consequence, various categories are formed of words with identical functions. This diversity comes from not only the lexicon frame but also the semantic frame of the construction. The prototype, in which both are fused, is schematized and categorized. In such a process the notion plays a large role. It is the same for foreign language teaching. Ungerer and Schmid (1996: 273) referred to the significance of the fusion of “form” (grammar) and “content” (meaning) for language teaching as follows:

“Yet no matter whether we choose a cognitive approach to grammar that is based on schemas, on prototypes or on basic level categories, there is one thing that is shared by all three approaches: they all manage to bridge the gap between formal syntax and morphology on the one hand and the semantic aspects of grammar on the other by relating them both to a common conceptual basis. This liberation from the form/context division is probably the most important

contribution that cognitive linguistics has made to pedagogical grammar and language teaching.”

4. Final Remarks

Section 2 referred to how the teacher should handle students’ incorrect expressions, which are apparently in neither the native language nor the target one. The author, adopting the concept of interlanguage, regards incorrect expressions as products of the learner’s psychological reaction and also suggests the necessity of conceptual instruction with little concern about syntactic aspects. Instruction under the integration of “meaning” and “form” based on a theoretical strategy of cognitive linguistic approach, like construction grammar, can be seen in the preceding section. If this strategy were applied to English teaching, instruction would likely be more effective.

Following Croft and Cruse (2004:229), in the most recent version of generative grammar, minimalist theory apparently ends the internal organization of the syntactic component and recasts the phonological component as an “articulatory-perceptual interface.” This links the language faculty to the perceptual-motor system, and the semantic component as a “conceptual-intentional interface.” This links the language faculty to other human conceptual activity. Thus, even in Chomskyan theory, where the syntactic component, separated from the semantic component, has been analyzed as the core of language knowledge, the integration of both theories begins to be seen.

Moreover, as Ungerer and Schmid (1996: 267) stated, it is true that “compared with linguistic structuralism, which sparked off substitution drills, and speech act theory, which initiated a complete reorganization of teaching strategies, the impact of cognitive linguistics is much less revolutionary. But the influence of cognitive linguistics may prove very valuable, because it lends theoretical support

(6)

19巻

to a number of accepted teaching approaches in the fields of both vocabulary and grammar”. It is the cognitive approach that bridges the gap between formal syntax and morphology on the one hand, and the semantic aspects of grammar on the other, by relating them both to a common conceptual basis. This approach liberates (English) language teaching from the form/content division based on the generative grammar. This statement is derived from cognitive grammar as an updated version of generative semantics. Basic principles of the latter were the same as the ones of the former. According to Lakoff (1987: 583), some of them are as follows:

―The primary function of language is to convey meaning. Grammar should therefore show as directly as possible how parameters of form are linked to parameters of meaning.

―Since meaning and communicative function are primary, grammar should attempt to explain as much as possible about parameters of form on the basis of parameters of meaning communicative function.

These principles are possibly related to the importance of instruction based on the integration of “meaning” and “form”. On the assumption that the concept of integration corresponds to that of interrelation, ideas regarding the interrelation between meaning and grammar which were suggested in Section 1, should be viewed under this concept of “integration”. Consequently we can see a certain theoretical basis as a preliminary finding to an actual verification of some ideas on the interrelation between meaning and grammar in English teaching in second-language learning.

Acknowledgement

The author would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for proofreading an earlier version of this paper and for his valuable comments. All shortcomings are mine.

(7)

Selected references

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspect of the Theory of

Syntax. Cambridge: MIT press

. (1986). Knowledge of Language. Westport: Praeger

Croft, W. (2001). Radical Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press

Croft, W. and Cruse, A. (2004). Cognitive

Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ.

Press

Ellis, R. (1997). Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press

Fillmore, C. J. (1988). The mechanisms of ‘Construction Grammar’. Berkeley Linguistic

Society, 14. 35-55

Fillmore, C. J. et al. (1988). Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: the case of let alone. Language 64. 501-538 Fried, M. What is Construction Grammar?

http://www.constructiongrammar.org/ webintro.htm〉

Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: a construction

grammar approach to argument structure.

Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press

. (1999). The Emergence of the Semantics of Argument Structure Constructions. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.) The

Emergence of Language. Mahwah: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates. 197-212

Hayase, N. and Horita, Y. (2005). Ninchibunpo

no shintenkai. Tokyo: Kenkyusha

Jae-Ho, L. (2001). Tadoshikobun no yuragigensho nikansuru ‘kobunteki’ apurochi. Papers in

Linguistic Science, 7. 1-21

Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous

Things. Chicago: Chicago Univ. Press

Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive

Grammar I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press

. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar II: Descriptive Application. Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press

. (1999). Grammar and

Conceptualization. Berlin: Mouton de

Gruyter

. (2000). Dynamic Usage-Based Model.

Usage-Based Models of Language, Barlow,

M. and Kemmer, S., 1-65, Stanford:CSLI Publications

Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological Foundations of

Language. Hoboken: John Wiley and Sons

Inc.

Mitchell, R. and Myles, F. (2004) Second

Language Learning Theories. (2nd ed.)

London: Hodder Arnold

Ohori, T. (2002). Introduction to the Science of

Language and Mind. Tokyo: Tokyo Univ.

Press

Ohori, T. et al. (2001). Kobunriron no genzai. The Rising Generation, Vol. 147 No.9, 526-549 Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International

Review of Applied linguistic, 10, 209-231.

Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a cognitive semantics

1: concept-structuring systems. Cambridge:

MIT Press

. (2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics

2: typology and process in concert-structuring.

Cambridge: MIT Press

Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language:

A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition.

Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press

Ueda, N. (2008). A Preliminary Study on the Correlation between Meaning and Grammar in Second-Language Learning. Morioka

Daigaku Tankidaidakubu Kiyo, 18, 57-67

Ungerer, F. and Schmid, H. (1996). An

Introduction to Cognitive Linguistics. London

and New York: Longman

Weinreich, U. (1953). Language in Contact. New York: The Linguistic Circle of New York Yamanashi, M. (1999). Cognitive Linguistics and

Japanese Language Education.

NIHONGO-KYOIKU TSUSHIN, 35, 9-11

. (2001). A Scenario for Linguistic Science ― From the Perspective of Cognitive Linguistics. Study in Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 1-28

参照

関連したドキュメント

We present sufficient conditions for the existence of solutions to Neu- mann and periodic boundary-value problems for some class of quasilinear ordinary differential equations.. We

In Section 13, we discuss flagged Schur polynomials, vexillary and dominant permutations, and give a simple formula for the polynomials D w , for 312-avoiding permutations.. In

Analogs of this theorem were proved by Roitberg for nonregular elliptic boundary- value problems and for general elliptic systems of differential equations, the mod- ified scale of

Then it follows immediately from a suitable version of “Hensel’s Lemma” [cf., e.g., the argument of [4], Lemma 2.1] that S may be obtained, as the notation suggests, as the m A

This paper presents an investigation into the mechanics of this specific problem and develops an analytical approach that accounts for the effects of geometrical and material data on

Correspondingly, the limiting sequence of metric spaces has a surpris- ingly simple description as a collection of random real trees (given below) in which certain pairs of

[Mag3] , Painlev´ e-type differential equations for the recurrence coefficients of semi- classical orthogonal polynomials, J. Zaslavsky , Asymptotic expansions of ratios of

[r]