• 検索結果がありません。

A ki koKochiyama The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading —Focusing on guessing the unknown word—

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

シェア "A ki koKochiyama The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading —Focusing on guessing the unknown word—"

Copied!
19
0
0

読み込み中.... (全文を見る)

全文

(1)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading

—Focusing on guessing the unknown word —

A ki ko Kochiyama

Introduction

In the foreign language classroom, it is frequently the case that students stop reading when they encounter "reading obstacles", such as unknown words or unfamiliar/ unmanageable content. Such reading "accidents" happen not only in the context of the classroom lesson, but also when students prepare for class (pre-reading or pre-study), read a newspaper, or even more seriously, during an exam where they can't use a dictionary . In many situations like these, students are likely to give up , feeling incapable of understanding unfamiliar and difficult material . It is a matter of course for the students to have these negative feelings of failure, which ultimately discourage them from reading further. The teacher's role in the language classroom then , is to encourage students not to give up, but to actively take on the text, that is, to be "active readers".

A main element of "active reading" is the use of strategies.

Therefore, it is crucial to provide students with the ability to

recognize and effectively use existing strategies, for example those

strategies which they use unconsciously when reading in their

native language. A possible approach is to activate and make

them aware of strategies for guessing the meaning of unknown

but necessary words. As Barnett writes, "Not all words are worth

guessing. Students begin to learn which words are minimally

(2)

important to text meaning by analyzing many in class" (1989, p.126). Focusing on those strategies for guessing necessary words, this study was conducted with the reader in the second language classroom in mind, seeking to uncover both the nature of the

strategies themselves and the actual process by which the reader uses these strategies.

Prior Research

Research on Reading

The two main studies focused on for this study were Smith (1985) on first language reading and Alderson (2000) on second language reading. Alderson (2000) divides reading into process, the actual act of reading which varies depending on the reader and the text, and product, the understanding a reader reaches after reading a text. This present study focuses on the reading process, defined by Smith (1985) as a process that answers "specific questions that we are asking"(96). If reading is variable and dynamic as Alderson says, then it would seem appropriate to focus on common strategies used by readers which were characterized as "knowing what questions to ask" when reading.

In defining reading as a "series of strategies and activities", Alderson (2000) identifies conscious strategies and automatic activities (i.e. identifying letters), and suggests two approaches to testing reading: the analytic approach (which tests aspects of the reading process that testers think are important) and the general approach (which views the entire reading process, because the act of testing a particular aspect of reading risks disturbing the reading process). The analytic approach was used in my research, to allow the focused testing of strategy activation and use in looking at conscious strategies for reading.

Smith (1985) and Alderson (2000) address the top-down

approach where meanings are understood before words and words

before letters, and the bottom-up approach where readers are

(3)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading S7

"passive decoders" fir

st of letters, then words, then meanings.

Although active reading is a part of the top-down approach , and would therefore seem to be more appropriate for my study, non- fluent readers may use the bottom-up approach. Therefore , it could be argued that reading may be more appropriately

characterized as an interactive model, where every part of the process of reading, whether it be linked to a top-down or bottom- up approach, interacts with all other parts.

Research on Reading Strategies

Research in the area of reading strategies began with the work of Hosenfeld (1977), a pioneer in this particular area . Based on subjects' self-reported data, she grouped strategies into strategies which lead to successful reading and strategies which lead to unsuccessful reading. In a further study in 1979, Hosenfeld used the "think-aloud" method and found that successful readers use some form of contextual guessing. She also reported at this time on one of the first attempts to train readers in the use of strategies. In addition to these first studies, particularly relevant to this present study is the list of 20 effective reading strategies, based on self-reports of adolescent foreign language students drawn up by Hosenfeld et al (1981) .

Although Hosenfeld did use the "think-aloud" method in her research, it was the work of Block (1986) which firmly established the use of think-aloud protocol. She classified learners into integrators and non-integrators, and grouped their strategies into two: general and local strategies. In addition to Block, Sarig (1987) also used think-aloud data to group the reading strategies of his subjects into four types.

Directly related to this present study is the work of Barnett

(1988) and Carrell (1989). Focusing on learner awareness of

reading strategy use, Barnett (1988) found that strategy users

understood more than readers who didn't use them, and readers

who were aware of their strategy use understood more than

(4)

readers who were not. In her turn, Carrell (1989) used questionnaires to analyze learners' awareness of strategy use, studying the relation between metacognitive awareness and reading. She grouped the subjects into readers who use global strategies and readers who use local ones, finding that the scores of the former were higher than those of the latter.

Finally, in one of the more recent studies on reading strategies, Takeuchi (2000) investigated the effect of the presence or absence of a reading task on subjects' responses to a reading strategy questionnaire. He found that when there was no attached reading task, the accuracy of subjects' responses concerning their actual strategy use decreased, and that subjects tended to overestimate the actual frequency of their strategy use. Given these findings, in the present study, subjects were asked to rate their strategy use with an attached reading task in an exercise which, though it is not in the questionnaire format, has the same purpose of gauging subjects' use of reading strategies.

Background of this Study

This present study is an expansion of a previous pilot study (Kochiyama 2000), and has as its starting point the list of effective reading strategies drawn up by Hosenfeld et al (1981). Based upon this list, I identified two groups of reading strategies:

grasping the main idea and guessing the meaning of unknown words. This latter category, guessing the meaning of unknown words, was then further subdivided into the following five components: 1) guessing strategies based on lexical knowledge (Stex): 2) guessing strategies based on grammatical knowledge (Sgr);

3) guessing strategies based on context (S.0; 4) guessing strategies based on previous knowledge (Spk); 5) guessing strategies based on information in the text, such as titles, illustrations/pictures, or notes (Sob).

For the purposes of this study, I chose to focus on the first three

(5)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading S9

strategies only, namely those based on lexical knowledge , grammatical knowledge, and context. The other strategies, namely four and five were not considered due to the large number of individual variables which made them difficult to determine to any level of objective accuracy.

Although Politzer and McGroarty (1985) point out that such individual variables as culture and personality amongst learners influence the effectiveness of a given strategy , the consideration of individual differences introduces many unaccountable variables, and may end up slowing down rather than helping the progress of research in the field. As Spiro and Myers (1987) appropriately stress, "every psychological component of reading , every aspect of a theory of reading, any way you can divide up the reading pie is a possible source of difference among individuals ."

Based on these considerations, I focused in my research on those variables that can be controlled, namely lexical, grammatical , and contextual strategies, which, as will be demonstrated , can be measured objectively in experimental tests.

To know how lexical, grammatical, and contextual guessing strategies behave and function in each stage of a learner's development is useful for giving learners advice and guidance in activating those strategies as tools for language learning , or more specifically as tools for guessing unknown words. Ideally , the progress of one learner would be followed as he/she became more proficient in the foreign or second language, observing the longitudinal growth in the learner's use of reading strategies , or more precisely their use of strategies for guessing unknown words . However, this would require research extending over a period of several years or more, and was not possible for the purposes of this study. To solve this problem, a large pool of data was collected

and analyzed using the methodology of "interlanguage analysis."

Based on the idea that large quantities of data from learners at various levels of L2 proficiency effectively reflect the progress of any one given individual, the methodology of "interlanguage

(6)

analysis" is particularly suited to the aim and objective of this study: to investigate the characteristics of lexical, grammatical, and contextual guessing strategies in the process of learners' development.

For the purposes of this study, lexical and grammatical strategies were combined into lexi-grammatical strategies, since lexical and grammatical strategies are both knowledge based.

Neither lexical nor grammatical rules/knowledge which are used in the native language are applicable in the foreign language; if the learner does not possess the given knowledge in the foreign language, these strategies cannot be transferred. In contrast, contextual strategies may be more readily transferred from the native language to the foreign language as they are not knowledge based. Due to this factor, lexi-grammatical strategies were considered as a unit, separate from contextual strategies.

With reference to prior research on reading, Alderson (2000) notes that non-fluent readers are "word-bound", indicating that non-fluent readers use more lexically-based and grammatically- based strategies. This in turn would therefore imply that fluent readers are more likely to use more context-based strategies.

However, I would like to suggest that the interactive model, which is a combination of the context-based top-down approach and the lexi-grammatical knowledge based bottom-up approach, most effectively describes the active reading process.

Research Question

How actively and successfully are contextual and lexi- grammatical strategies used by foreign language readers at different proficiency levels?

(7)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 91

Experiment

Comparison of contextual and lexi-grammatical strategies

Testing the activation of lexi-grammatical strategies, the first part contains 30 questions, of which 20 directly test the use of lexi-grammatical strategies, and 10 are dummy questions. The 20 experimental questions are modified by the tester from questions from the STEP test level 2, and are made so that only lexi-grammatical strategies can be used in order to determine the meaning of the unknown word, represented as a blank in the

question. This is done by isolating a sentence from the context of other sentences. By so doing all endophoric referencing is denied the reader and as such he/she is placed in a position of having to rely more on lexi-grammatical strategies to make a meaningful

choice. For example, if one looks at the following example taken from question 2:

(2) My uncle only visits us ( ) .

1) immediate 2) eventual 3) actual

4) occasionally In the above example, the reader cannot make reference, whether it be anaphoric or cataphoric, to any text surrounding the sentence or indeed to any exophoric reference to make sense of who My uncle or us refers to. Instead the reader must make their choice of occasionally based on their grammatical knowledge of the positioning of adverbs and adjectives in a sentence, as well as their lexical knowledge of the meaning of the word "only". As mentioned earlier, and as demonstrated in this question, lexical strategies and grammatical strategies are usually combined very tightly, and it is difficult to make questions which test each strategy separately. Therefore, lexical strategies and grammatical strategies are tested together as "lexi-grammatical" strategies.

Randomly mixed in with the 20 lexi-grammatical questions, the 10 dummy questions are taken directly from the STEP test

(8)

level 2. These dummy questions are used to insure that the subjects do not automatically use lexi-grammatical strategies thinking that this part tests only lexi-grammatical strategy use, but consciously choose which strategies to use for each question.

After a few questions, subjects would notice that to solve these questions, they need only use lexi-grammatical knowledge, without thinking about the meaning of the question (using contextual strategies), and their thinking patterns would become set in the "lexi-grammatical" mode. Therefore 10 dummy questions are used to avoid this. The subjects' answers to these dummy questions are later used as a portion of their proficiency test, and only the answers to the 20 lexi-grammatical questions are used as a measurement of lexi-grammatical strategy activation.

Following the test of lexi-grammatical strategy activation is a test of contextual strategy activation using four longer passages, each of which is approximately 300 words. These passages are mainly taken from the STEP test level 2 given in spring of the year 2000, and like the test of lexi-grammatical strategy activation, 10 of the 30 questions are dummy questions taken directly from the STEP test level 2. The remaining 20 questions are experimental questions modified by the tester. Here

again, the answers to the dummy questions are later used as a part of the subjects' proficiency test. The 20 questions testing contextual strategy activation are created so that lexical knowledge cannot be used, by making all of the answer choices very basic words which subjects should already know. Therefore,

subjects would not find it useful to apply their knowledge of suffixes and prefixes or meanings of root words. To ensure that grammatical knowledge is used less than contextual knowledge, all of the four answer choices are of the same grammatical form.

Given this, the reader is placed in a position of having to choose

an answer based more upon the references he/she can find from

the immediate surrounding text. For example, if we look at

question 43:

(9)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 93

All over the house, TV screens and computer sensors ( ) your movements and desires. If you move from room to room, the computer will automatically transfer the movie you were watching to the ( 43 ) screen. Heaters and air conditioners work the same way, only heating or cooling rooms with people in them and following people as they move.

43) 1) distant 2) nearest 3) far 4) away

In this example, the reader is faced with four choices, three of which could fit if one applied the grammar rule that all adjectives precede the noun. However, using this approach would quite obviously not yield the correct answer, and instead the reader is placed in a position of having to make a meaningful choice using the preceding and subsequent sentences. In this example , the context is one of making domestic gadgetry more convenient . Using this context, the reader is guided to choosing nearest as their answer.

In order to further validate the reliability of the above tests (lexi-grammatical strategy activation test and contextual strategy activation test), the subjects are asked to think about what cues they used and for what reason they used those cues in answering the preceding questions. First, as a brainstorming activity , subjects are asked what they do when they come across an unknown word in a passage, and there is no dictionary available . The second question asks the subjects to list all the cues they could think of for guessing the meaning of unknown words. After this, subjects are given a list of five clues for guessing the meaning of unknown words which correspond to the five strategies (Siex.

Sgr, Scon Spk, Soh), and they are asked to go back through the preceding questions and write which clue they mainly used of the five clues given in answering each of the questions. Although this task of identifying which strategy they used in answering the questions is not directly in the questionnaire format, its

(10)

validity is supported by the research of Takeuchi (2000) on the use of questionnaires in strategy research. Finding that subjects who were given a questionnaire on strategy use with an attached reading task were less likely to overestimate their actual use of strategies when reading, his study supports my use of the strategy identification task in conjunction with the test of contextual strategy activation.

In the preceding questions, which are in the more controlled form of multiple-choice, it is difficult to see the variety of individual guessing processes. To account for this, subjects are next asked to pick out the words they did not know from the final two passages

of the test of contextual strategy activation, and to guess the meanings of these words. By doing so, the subjects are given more freedom in their choices, and the tester can observe the

"real picture" of the guessing process

, although this is more difficult to analyze statistically. This type of data is most suited for qualitative analysis.

After underlining the unknown words, they are asked to judge the importance of each underlined word, based on whether

guessing the meaning of this unknown word is very necessary for grasping the main idea of the whole passage, i.e. which words must be guessed and which may be skipped. Necessary words are marked with a circle, while unnecessary words are marked with an x. Subjects are asked only to guess the meaning of necessary words (marked with a circle), and to write down the guessed meaning under these words as well as which clues they used in their guessing. They are asked to write down which clues they used in order of their importance. This allows the tester to see which strategies are used when guessing the meaning of each unknown word, but to see how much each strategy is used when guessing, the subjects are asked to choose four words from among the necessary words (marked with a circle) that they consider to be the most important. On a scale of one to ten, they are asked to rate the clues they used in guessing these words (if they used a

(11)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 95

clue very much, it would be rated a "10"). From this , the tester can see not only which are strategies are used, but also how much each strategy is used when guessing the meaning of an unknown word.

Results and Discussion

Results of the multiple-choice questions

40

ar 35 w, 30 CIJas oc 25

ar 20 :: 15

jto

5

0 Ci

ar

E

t

III

0

00

y.r ar

"O

ar

e

IC;

ION b

. WI 0 b.

ar Cos 40 v1 ea ea

English Proficiency

a OG

z tti

MI

sr i y

;A0oar

w ~ ~

sa

Others

Strategies based on given hints

and others O Contextual

strategies

^ Grammatical strategies

© Lexical strategies

Figure 1: Results of the multiple-choice test.

In the above figure, the five strategies (lexical , grammatical, contextual, given hints, and others) are

represented as gradations from white to black from the

bottom to the top, with white for lexical strategies (bottom)

and black for others (top). For each proficiency level

(elementary, intermediate, advanced), the left and right

columns indicate the degree of activation and successful

activation, respectively. N=(208,180,180)

(12)

Figure 1 shows the results of the multiple-choice test. For each proficiency level, the left column provides information on the number of times a strategy was used, indicated by a specific color (white for lexical strategies for example), and the right column indicates the degree of success with which a reading strategy has been successfully activated, measured by the number of correct answers given. Recall that in total, 40 questions were given in the multiple-choice test. It can be seen from Figure 1, therefore, that the total number of successfully answered questions increases with proficiency. Also, from Figure 1, lexi-grammatical strategies are activated more often for elementary than for advanced levels, but the number of items successfully activated is smaller.

Moreover, both the degree of activation and success in activation for contextual strategies are significantly larger for the advanced proficiency level. Note that for "others", represented by the color black in Figure 1, there is a drastic decrease in use as the students' proficiency improves. This is to be expected since more proficient

candidates have a broader range of knowledge to drawn upon to answer the questions. That is, they are less preoccupied with word level technicalities and are better able to focus on global meaning.

From Figure 1, the following statements are clear.

In the multiple-choice test:

1) As the proficiency level increases, the average number

of times contextual strategies are used increases

compared with lexi-grammatical strategies.

2) As the proficiency level increases, the average number

of times contextual strategies are successfully used increases compared with lexi-grammatical strategies.

3) As the proficiency level increases, the success rate of contextual strategy use increases compared with lexi-

grammatical strategies.

(13)

The Infuence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 97

Results of the multiple-choice questions (successfully)

16.00

14.00 12.00

10.00

8.00

8.00 4.00

2.00

0.00

Elementary (

Figure 1'

N =208)

Number

Intermediate ( N —180)

of times each

Advanced( N =180)

of the strategies is used.

Results of the multiple-choice questions 20.00

18.00 18.00 14.00 12.00 10.00 8.00 8.00 4.00 2.00 0.00

Elementary(

Figure 1"

N =208) Intermediate( N =180)

Number of times each successfully used.

-•— Lexical strategies

f— grammatical strategies

--d^- Contextual strategies

- Strategies

based on given hints and others

Advanced( N =180)

of the strategies is

(14)

These graphs were made based on Figure 1. From these graphs, we can compare clearly the number of times each of the five strategies is used. Figure 1' shows the number of times each of the strategies is used, while Figure 1" shows the number of times each of the strategies is successfully used. It is very clear that as the proficiency level increases, contextual strategies are used more frequently and also more successfully than lexi- grammatical strategies.

Through these graphs, statements 1, 2, and 3 given above are clearly reconfirmed, because in these figures we can compare the concrete number of times each strategy is used. Concerning statement 1, the number of times contextual strategies are used greatly increases as shown by the triangular curve, while lexi- grammatical strategy use does not increase to the extent of contextual strategy use, as shown by the square and diamond curves. Also concerning statement 2, the same tendency is observed. For statement 3, the difference in the steepness of the triangular curves between 1' and 1" shows that the success rate increases as the proficiency level increases. The same can also be said for the square and diamond curves. Regarding learner awareness of which strategies were used, it was determined that advanced learners were able to better identify which strategies they needed to answer the questions accurately, and so therefore in their self-report on their own strategy use, were better able to assess what strategies they in fact used. On the other hand, elementary learners could not identify which strategies they used, so they assessed their strategies to be in group 5 (given hints and others, others meaning mainly lucky guesses). Even though the number at the elementary level for the number of times strategies in group 5 were used is very large in Figure 1', the number of times the same strategies were used in Figure 1" is very small.

This means that strategies in group 5 mainly consisted of lucky guesses, as the success rate is extremely low. This implies that the reality of strategies in group 5 which were reported by the subjects were just lucky guesses, since in this particular test

(15)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 99

situation, the passages did pictures or titles.

not have any given hints such as

Results of the free description questions

a w a. t w ot 0 I. a.

4. 0 u 0 ar a

L Lao

10 9 8 7 8 5 4 3 2 1 0

4 4+ CC C 44

E MOM

6R Li 3 cA C 0 ar

•f. 11;

a cS 0 4 M. C.

0

•rC

h aJ G SS 0

40 4 ar 4 4 0 u C

ar W 0 44

E 4.

C

VI as va C OS CLi 0 A. 0 4 C.

Q C

con 4 a,

C

^..

0 as 4 I. C U C

b ar N C of lit

CA 4

0

C ..

C.

0 A. C.

C.

al a

on 4 44

3 C 0 r+ ti

as 4 4 C cr C

® Strategy based on given hints and others

0 Contextual Strategies

0 Grammatical strategies

0 Lexical strategies

Figure 2 Frequency of strategy activation in the free description test. In the above figure, the five strategies (lexical , grammatical, contextual, given hints, and others) are represented as gradations from white to black from the bottom to the top, with white for lexical strategies (bottom) and black for others (top). For each proficiency level (elementary, intermediate, advanced) , the figure shows the frequency of strategy activation (left column) , almost successful activation (central column), and successful activation (right column). Note that the total frequency of activation is different for each proficiency level because the number of unknown words is different .

Figure 2 shows the results of the free description tests . The graph is similar to the one presented in Figure 1 except that one

(16)

extra central column has been added for each proficiency level, and this shows the number of activated strategies which were

almost successful, that is, strategies which led to appropriate answers (to clarify further, correct and almost correct answers).

This category was included because it was deemed necessary due to the style of the test, which was less restrictive than a multiple- choice test, allowing the student to answer in a more qualitative manner. Here, the bar represents the number of strategies used for guessing unknown words that the subjects believe to be important. For the elementary subjects, this number is smaller than for the intermediate subjects, probably reflecting the overwhelming number of unknown words for them. This is further supported by the fact that the number of words unassociated with any strategy is considerably larger for beginners (2.5, 1, and 0.5, respectively). It can also be seen that the relative frequency of successful activation increases with the increasing level proficiency of the subjects.

From Figure 2, we can conclude as follows:

1) As the proficiency level increases, the average number of times contextual strategies are used increases compared

with lexi-grammatical strategies.

2) As the proficiency level increases, the average number of times contextual strategies are almost successfully used

(appropriate answers) increases compared with lexi-

grammatical strategies.

3) As the proficiency level increases, the average number of times contextual strategies are successfully used (correct

answers) increases compared with lexi-grammatical

strategies.

Conclusion and Implications for Teaching

In response to the research question, "How actively and successfully are contextual and lexi-grammatical strategies used by EFL readers at different proficiency levels?", we can conclude

(17)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 101

the following: With higher levels of language proficiency, foreign language readers use contextual strategies more actively and more successfully than lexi-grammatical strategies. At lower levels of language proficiency, readers tend to rely more on lexi- grammatical strategies alone.

These results point to a number of implications for the teaching of reading to foreign language students. As it was found that as the proficiency level increases the rate at which the subjects use contextual strategies is greater when compared with lexi-grammatical strategies, this means that to be an advanced student, contextual strategies are crucial. Thus, one important implication for teaching is that, to improve students' reading proficiency, effective student use of contextual strategies needs to be promoted. Accordingly, in reading instruction classes , more emphasis should be placed on the teaching of contextual strategies . Furthermore, instructors of English as a second language could make better use of the ability of advanced learners to use contextual strategies, by integrating new words in several different contexts so as to further sharpen learners' awareness of contextual strategies. This is in great contrast to the learning approach based simply on intellectual power where a student merely tries to memorize a vocabulary list.

Turning to the implications for elementary learners, the same suggestion could be made. Due to elementary learners' tendency to favor the use of lexi-grammatical strategies over contextual ones, these too could be better utilized in teaching practice. For example, word-based information, such as similarities between words, synonyms, and antonyms, could be more effectively exploited to provide learners with concrete ways of making more immediate sense of texts, thereby increasing the motivation students have for reading.

(18)

References

Alderson, J.C. (2000). Assessing Reading. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Barnett, M.A. (1988a). Following reference words. One aspect of reading comprehension. Unpublished manuscript.

Barnett, M. A. (1988b). Reading through context: How real and perceived

strategy use affects L2 comprehension. Modern Language Journal, 72(2), 150-62.

Barnett, M. A. (1988c). Teaching reading strategies: How methodology

affects language course articulation. Foreign Language Annals, 21(2), 109-19.

Block, E. (1986). The comprehension strategies of second language

readers. TESOL Quarterly, 20(3), 463-94.

Carrell, P. L. (1989). Metacognitive awareness and second language

reading. The Modern Language Journal, 73(2).

Carrell, P.L. (1991). Second language reading: Reading ability or language proficiency? Applied Linguistics 12/2, 159-179.

Hosenfeld, C. (1977b). A preliminary investigation of the reading strategies of successful and nonsuccessful second language learners.

System, 5(2), 110-23.

Hosenfeld, C. (1979a). Cindy: A learner in today's foreign language

classroom. In W. C. Born(Ed.), The Foreign Language Learner in Today's Classroom Environment. Middlebury, VT: Northeast

Conference, 53-75. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service, No. ED 185 837).

Hosenfeld, C. (1979W. A learning-teaching view of second language

instruction. Foreign Language Annals, 12, 51-54.

Hosenfeld, C., Arnold, V, Kirchofer, J., Laciura, J., and Wilson, L., (1981).

Second language reading: A curricular sequence for teaching reading

strategies. Foreign Language Annals, 14(5), 415-22.

Kochiyama, A. (2000). An empirical study on context-based strategies

for guessing a word's meaning. JACET Bulletin, Vol. 32, 63-77.

Politzer, R.L., & McGroarty, M. (1985). An exploratory study of learning

behaviors and their relationship to gains in linguistic and communicative competence. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 103-123.

(19)

The Influence of Proficiency Level on Learners' Reading 103

Sarig, G. (1987). High-level reading in the first and in the foreign language: Some comparative process data. In J. Devine, P. L.

Carrell, & D. E. Eskey (Eds.), Research in Reading in English as a

Second Language. Washington, DC: Teachers of English to Speakers

of Other Languages, 107-120.

Smith, F. (1985). Reading without Nonsense. Columbia University, NewYork: Teachers College Press.

Spiro, R.J. and Myers, A. (1984). Individual differences and underlying

cognitive processes in reading. In Pearson, P.D. et al. (Eds.), Handbook ofReadingResearch. New York: Longman, 471-504.

Takeuchi, O. (2000). Shitsumonshiho to gaikokugo gakushu horyaku:

kenkyushuho no kanten kara. (The questionnaire method and language learning strategies: From the perspective of research

method). Kotoba no Kagaku Kenkyu, Vol. 1, 67-81.

Tenma, M. (1989). Eibun Dokkai no Strategy. (Strategies for English Reading Competence). Tokyo: Taishukan Shoten.

Wenden, A. (1991). Learner Srategies for Learner Autonomy. New Jersey:

Prentice Hall.

Figure   1: Results  of  the  multiple-choice  test.
Figure   2  Frequency  of  strategy  activation  in  the  free  description  test.  In  the  above  figure,  the  five  strategies  (lexical ,  grammatical,  contextual,  given  hints,  and  others)  are  represented  as  gradations  from  white  to  black

参照

関連したドキュメント

It is suggested by our method that most of the quadratic algebras for all St¨ ackel equivalence classes of 3D second order quantum superintegrable systems on conformally flat

This paper is devoted to the investigation of the global asymptotic stability properties of switched systems subject to internal constant point delays, while the matrices defining

In this paper, we focus on the existence and some properties of disease-free and endemic equilibrium points of a SVEIRS model subject to an eventual constant regular vaccination

Yin, “Global existence and blow-up phenomena for an integrable two-component Camassa-Holm shallow water system,” Journal of Differential Equations, vol.. Yin, “Global weak

We study the classical invariant theory of the B´ ezoutiant R(A, B) of a pair of binary forms A, B.. We also describe a ‘generic reduc- tion formula’ which recovers B from R(A, B)

For X-valued vector functions the Dinculeanu integral with respect to a σ-additive scalar measure on P (see Note 1) is the same as the Bochner integral and hence the Dinculeanu

Moreover, by (4.9) one of the last two inequalities must be proper.. We briefly say k-set for a set of cardinality k. Its number of vertices |V | is called the order of H. We say that

Finally, in Figure 19, the lower bound is compared with the curves of constant basin area, already shown in Figure 13, and the scatter of buckling loads obtained